Skip Navigation
College Drinking Prevention - Changing the Culture

Stats & Summaries NIAAA College Materials Supporting Research Other Alcohol Information NewSpecial Features
College Presidents College Parents College Students H.S. Administrators H.S. Parents & Students
Supporting Research

Journal of Studies on Alcohol

College Drinking Statistical Papers

Funding

Related Research

 
Helpful Tools

In the News

Join Our Listserv

Links

Order Publications

Link to Us

E-mail this Page

Print this Page


Effects of Minimum Drinking Age Laws: Review and Analyses of the Literature from 1960 to 2000

ALEXANDER C. WAGENAAR, Ph.D., andTRACI L. TOOMEY, Ph.D.
Division of Epidemiology, School of Public Health, University of Minnesota, 1300 South Second Street, Suite 300, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55454-1015

ABSTRACT. Objective: The goal of this article is to review critically the extant minimum legal drinking age (MLDA) research literature and summarize the current state of knowledge regarding the effectiveness of this policy. Method: Comprehensive searches of four databases were conducted to identify empirical studies of the MLDA published from 1960 to 1999. Three variables were coded for each study regarding methodological quality: (1) sampling design, (2) study design and (3) presence or absence of comparison group. Results: We identified 241 empirical analyses of the MLDA. Fifty-six percent of the analyses met our criteria for high methodological quality. Of the 33 higher quality studies of MLDA and alcohol consumption, 11 (33%) found an inverse relationship; only 1 found the opposite. Similarly, of the 79 higher quality analyses of MLDA and traffic crashes, 46 (58%) found a higher MLDA related to decreased traffic crashes; none found the opposite. Eight of the 23 analyses of other problems found a higher MLDA associated with reduced problems; none found the opposite. Only 6 of the 64 college-specific studies (9%) were of high quality; none found a significant relationship between the MLDA and outcome measures. Conclusions: The preponderance of evidence indicates there is an inverse relationship between the MLDA and two outcome measures: alcohol consumption and traffic crashes. The quality of the studies of specific populations such as college students is poor, preventing any conclusions that the effects of MLDA might differ for such special populations. (J. Stud. Alcohol, Supplement No. 14: 206-225, 2002)


THE MINIMUM legal drinking age (MLDA) is the most well-studied alcohol control policy in the United States (Wagenaar and Toomey, 2000). The intention of this policy is to lower alcohol use and its associated problems among youth. Following Prohibition, most states established an age-21 MLDA. During the early 1970s, a trend toward lowering the MLDA to age 18, 19 or 20 began in the United States, providing many natural experiments. As a result of research evidence indicating that traffic crashes among youth increased following lowering of the legal age, a citizens' effort began urging states to raise the MLDA back to age 21. In 1984, the federal government enacted the Uniform Drinking Age Act, which provided for the withholding of federal highway funds from states that failed to increase their MLDA (King and Dudar, 1987). By 1988, all states had established an age-21 MLDA. The increase in MLDA across multiple states again provided researchers with many natural experiments to assess effects of these policy changes on alcohol consumption and related problems among youth.

Despite this long history, the debate over the MLDA continues. Part of this debate is whether the age-21 MLDA is really effective in reducing alcohol-related problems. This debate is particularly relevant to college campuses because the majority of students on many campuses are under age 21. Some college administrators argue that the age-21 law has caused more problems on college campuses, not less (Lonnstrom, 1985).

To determine the overall effect of the age-21 MLDA on youth, including college-age students, the existing research literature should be critically reviewed. The purpose of this review is to summarize all studies available in the peer-reviewed published literature over the past four decades that evaluated the effects of public policies establishing a legal minimum age for purchase and/or consumption of alcoholic beverages. Most studies assessed effects of the MLDA on consumption and alcohol-related problems among all those under age 21—college students and those not in college. Some MLDA studies specifically assessed effects of MLDA changes on college students alone. Given the current discussions on college campuses, we provide a review of the college studies in addition to a summary of the overall MLDA literature. A second objective of this article is to describe key issues in public debates regarding MLDA policies.

Method

[Table 1]

We obtained all identified published studies on the drinking age from 1960 to 1999, a total of 132 documents. Comprehensive searches were conducted of four databases to identify studies of interest: ETOH (1960-1999 [National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism's alcohol and alcohol problems science database]), MEDLINE (1966-1999), Current Contents (1994-1999) and Social Science Abstracts (1983-1999). The entire record for each document was included in the search; thus, any record with any search term in the title, keywords, subject headings, descriptors or abstract fields was identified. Search terms used for each database were as follows (where * is the truncation indicator to include all forms of the root word):

  • ETOH: (minimum age OR drinking age OR purchase age OR legal age OR MDA OR MLDA) OR ([teen* OR adolescen* OR young OR college* OR youth* OR student* OR underage* OR minor*] AND [sale* OR enforce* OR deterrence* OR avail* OR access* OR crackdown OR ID OR identification OR compliance])
  • MEDLINE and Current Contents: (minimum age OR drinking age OR purchase age OR legal age OR MLDA) OR ([teen* OR adolescen* OR young OR college* OR youth OR student* OR underage* OR minor*] AND [sale* OR enforce* OR deterrence* OR avail* OR access* OR crackdown OR ID OR identification OR compliance])
  • Social Science Abstracts: (minimum age OR drinking age OR purchase age OR legal age OR MDA OR MLDA)

In addition, two previous literature reviews were used to identify relevant studies (Wagenaar, 1983a, 1993).

We obtained and reviewed the original document for each study and coded eight key variables for each study. These variables include the jurisdiction studied (i.e., state or province), specific outcome measures analyzed (e.g., self-reported drinking, car crash fatalities) and whether the study was specific to college student populations. In addition, three key indicators of methodological quality were coded for each study. The first is sampling design, distinguishing lower quality nonprobability sampling versus higher quality probability sampling or census data. The second quality indicator is the research or study design, with lower quality studies consisting of cross-sectional (one time-point) observations only versus higher quality studies that used pre-post (one observation before a policy change and one after), longitudinal (more than 2 but fewer than 20 repeated observations) or time-series (20 or more repeated observations over time) designs. The third quality indicator is whether some form of comparison group was used; studies with no comparison groups are of low quality. Finally, we coded whether the findings were statistically significant. If the results were significant, we coded the direction of the relationship between legal age for drinking and a specific outcome measure.

Effects of drinking age on alcohol consumption

We located 48 published studies that assessed the effects of changes in the legal minimum drinking age on indicators of alcohol consumption (Table 1). In the 48 studies, a total of 78 alcohol consumption outcome measures were analyzed (e.g., sales figures, self-reported drinking). Of the 78 analyses, 27 (35%) found a statistically significant inverse relationship between the legal drinking age and alcohol consumption; that is, as the legal age was lowered, drinking increased, and as the legal age was raised, drinking decreased. An additional 8 analyses that found an inverse relationship did not report significance levels. Of the 78 analyses, only 5 found a positive relationship between the legal drinking age and consumption. In short, 45% of all analyses found that a higher legal drinking age is associated with reduced alcohol consumption.

[Table 2]

Of the 78 analyses of alcohol consumption, 21 were the weaker cross-sectional designs, and 57 were pre-post, longitudinal or time-series designs. Of the 21 cross-sectional analyses, 8 (38%) found a significant inverse relationship between legal drinking age and alcohol consumption, whereas only 3 found a significant positive relationship. An additional 4 analyses found an inverse relationship, and 1 found a positive relationship; however, significance levels were not reported. Of the 57 longitudinal analyses (i.e., which we define as any analyses that included repeated measures over time), 19 (33%) found a significant inverse relationship; only 1 longitudinal study found a significant positive relationship. An additional 4 longitudinal analyses found an inverse relationship but did not report significance levels.

Of the 78 analyses of alcohol consumption, 55 (71%) included a comparison group of some kind. For 3 analyses, it was not clear whether a comparison group was used (not avail.). Of the 55 analyses including comparison groups, 23 (42%) found a significant inverse relationship; only 4 found a significant positive relationship. An additional 3 analyses found an inverse relationship, and 1 analysis found a positive relationship but no significance levels were reported. Of the 20 analyses that did not include comparison groups, 4 found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and alcohol consumption, and none found a positive relationship. An additional 4 analyses without comparison groups found an inverse relationship but did not report significance levels.

Of the 78 analyses of alcohol consumption, 58 (74%) included probability samples or a complete census of the relevant population, and 11 analyses clearly did not use a probability sample or census. For an additional 9 analyses, it was unclear whether a probability sample or census was used. Of the 58 with a probability sample or census, 20 (34%) found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and alcohol consumption; only 1 study found a significant positive relationship. An additional 8 studies found an inverse relationship but did not report significance levels, and 26 analyses found no significant relationship. Of the 11 analyses without a probability sample or census, 2 found a significant inverse relationship, and 3 found a significant positive relationship. One additional study found a positive relationship but did not report significance. Of the 9 analyses for which it was unclear whether a probability sample or census was used, 5 found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and alcohol consumption; none found a significant positive relationship.

Finally, of the 78 analyses of alcohol consumption, only 24 were specific to college student populations. Of the 24 college-specific analyses, 3 (13%) found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and alcohol consumption, 3 found a significant positive relationship, and 15 found no significant relationship. One additional study found an inverse relationship with no report on significance levels. Of the 54 analyses that were not college specific, 24 (44%) found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and alcohol consumption. Only 1 found a significant positive relationship. An additional 7 analyses found an inverse relationship, and 1 found a positive relationship but did not report significance levels.

In conclusion, the preponderance of evidence suggests that higher legal drinking ages reduce alcohol consumption. Of all analyses that reported significant effects, 87% found higher drinking ages associated with lower alcohol consumption. Only 13% found the opposite. The evidence is not entirely consistent: Almost half (46%) of the analyses found no association between the legal age and indicators of alcohol consumption. However, focusing on the 33 of the 78 studies of high methodological quality (i.e., those that include a longitudinal design, comparison groups and probability sampling or use of a census) reveals that 11 (33%) of the 33 higher quality studies found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and alcohol consumption. Only 1 (3%) found a significant positive relationship. Only 3 of these studies of higher quality were college specific, and results were not significant in all 3 studies.

Effects of drinking age on driving after drinking and traffic crashes

We located 57 published studies that assessed the effects of changes in the legal minimum drinking age on indicators of driving after drinking and traffic crashes (Table 2). In the 57 studies, a total of 102 crash outcome measures were analyzed (e.g., fatal crashes, drink-driving crashes, self-reported driving after drinking). Of the 102 analyses, 52 (51%) found a statistically significant inverse relationship between the legal drinking age and crashes; that is, as the legal age was lowered, the number of crashes increased, and as the legal age was raised, the number of crashes decreased. (From here on, we use the term crashes to include all traffic-related outcome measures.) An additional 12 analyses that found an inverse relationship did not report significance levels. Of the 102 analyses, only 2 found a positive relationship between the legal drinking age and traffic crashes. In short, more than half of all analyses found that a higher legal drinking age is associated with decreased rates of traffic crashes.

Of the 102 analyses of traffic crashes, 14 were the weaker cross-sectional designs, and 88 were longitudinal designs. Of the 14 cross-sectional analyses, 5 (36%) found a significant inverse relationship between legal drinking age and crashes, whereas only 1 found a significant positive relationship. Of the 88 longitudinal analyses, 47 (53%) found a significant inverse relationship; none found a significant positive relationship. An additional 12 found an inverse relationship, and 1 found a positive relationship but did not report significance levels.

Of the 102 analyses of traffic crashes, 95 (93%) included a comparison group of some kind (for 2 analyses it was not clear whether a comparison group was used). Of the 95 analyses including comparison groups, 50 (53%) found a significant inverse relationship; only 1 found a significant positive relationship. An additional 11 analyses found an inverse relationship but no significance levels were reported. Of the 5 analyses that did not include comparison groups, 1 found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and traffic crashes. One additional analysis without comparison groups found an inverse relationship, and 1 found a positive relationship but did not report significance levels.

Of the 102 analyses of traffic crashes, 94 (92%) included probability samples or a complete census of the relevant population, and 3 analyses clearly did not use a probability sample or census. For an additional 5 analyses it was unclear whether a probability sample or census was used. Of the 94 with a probability sample or census, 49 (52%) found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and traffic crashes; only 1 study found a significant positive relationship. An additional 11 studies found an inverse relationship, and 1 study found a positive relationship but did not report significance levels; 34 analyses found no significant relationship. Of the 3 analyses without a probability sample or census, 2 found a significant inverse relationship, and none found a significant positive relationship. Of the 5 analyses for which it was unclear whether a probability sample or census was used, 1 found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and traffic crashes; none found a significant positive relationship.

Finally, of the 102 analyses of traffic crashes, only 6 were specific to college student populations. Of the 6 college-specific analyses, 2 (33%) found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and traffic crashes, 1 found a positive relationship but significance was not reported, and 3 found no significant relationship. Of the 96 analyses that were not college specific, 50 (52%) found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and traffic crashes; only 1 found a significant positive relationship. An additional 12 analyses found an inverse relationship but did not report significance levels.

In conclusion, the preponderance of evidence indicates that higher legal drinking ages reduce rates of traffic crashes. Of all analyses that reported significant effects, 98% found higher drinking ages associated with lower rates of traffic crashes. Only 2% found the opposite. The evidence, however, is not entirely consistent: 35% of the analyses found no association between the legal age and indicators of traffic crashes. However, focusing on the 79 studies of higher methodological quality (i.e., those that include a longitudinal design, comparison groups and probability sampling or use of a census) reveals that 46 (58%) of these 79 higher quality studies found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and traffic crashes; none found a significant positive relationship. None of these studies of higher quality were college specific.

[Table 3]

Effects of drinking age on other health and social problem outcomes

We identified 24 published studies that assessed the effects of changes in the legal minimum drinking age on indicators of other health and social problem outcomes (other than traffic crashes), such as suicide, homicide or vandalism (Table 3). In the 24 studies, 61 outcome measures were analyzed. Of the 61 analyses, 10 (16%) found a statistically significant inverse relationship between the legal drinking age and other outcomes; that is, as the legal age was lowered, the number of problems increased, and as the legal age was raised, the number of problems decreased. Of the 61 analyses, 4 found a positive relationship between the legal drinking age and other outcomes; an additional 2 analyses that found an inverse relationship and 1 that found a positive relationship did not report significance levels.

Of the 61 analyses of other health and social problems, 16 were the weaker cross-sectional designs, and 45 were longitudinal designs. Of the 16 cross-sectional analyses, 1 (6%) found a significant inverse relationship between legal drinking age and other problems; none found a significant positive relationship. Of the 45 longitudinal analyses, 9 (20%) found a significant inverse relationship; 3 found a significant positive relationship.

Of the 61 analyses of other health and social problems, 36 (59%) included a comparison group of some kind (for 4 analyses it was not clear whether a comparison group was used). Of the 36 analyses including comparison groups, 9 (25%) found a significant inverse relationship; none found a significant positive relationship. One additional analysis found a positive relationship but no significance levels were reported. Of the 25 analyses that did not include comparison groups, 1 found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and other problems, and 3 found a positive relationship.

Of the 61 analyses of other problems, 47 (77%) included probability samples or a complete census of the relevant population, 12 analyses clearly did not use a probability sample or census, and for an additional 2 analyses it was unclear whether a probability sample or census was used. Of the 47 with a probability sample or census, 9 (19%) found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and other problems; only 2 studies found a significant positive relationship. An additional 2 studies found an inverse relationship but did not report significance levels, and 33 analyses found no significant relationship. Of the 12 analyses without a probability sample or census, 1 found a significant inverse relationship, and 1 found a significant positive relationship. Of the 2 analyses for which it was unclear whether a probability sample or census was used, neither found significant inverse or positive relationships between the legal age and other problems.

[Table 4]

Finally, of the 61 analyses of other health and social problems, 34 were specific to college student populations. Of the 34 college-specific analyses, 2 (6%) found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and other health and social problems, and 3 found a significant positive relationship. Two additional studies found an inverse relationship with no report on significance levels. Of the 27 analyses that were not college specific, 8 (30%) found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and other problems; none found a significant positive relationship. One additional analysis found a positive relationship but did not report significance levels.

In conclusion, although there is clearly some evidence that higher legal drinking ages reduce rates of other health and social problems, results are not as consistent as they are for traffic crash outcome indicators. Of all analyses that reported significant effects, 75% found higher drinking ages associated with lower rates of problems. Only 25% found the opposite. The evidence, however, is not entirely consistent: 72% of the analyses found no association between the legal age and indicators of other problems. However, focusing on the 23 studies of higher methodological quality (i.e., those that include a longitudinal design, comparison groups and probability sampling or use of a census) reveals that 8 (35%) of the 23 higher quality studies found a significant inverse relationship between the legal age and other problems; none found a significant positive relationship. Two of those studies of higher quality were college specific; however, results of both were not significant.

Given the diverse types of outcomes included in this section, we also stratified the analyses of the higher methodological studies into four groups that were more homogeneous. Of the 16 analyses of nontraffic injuries (fatal and nonfatal), 4 found a significant inverse relationship between the legal drinking age and injuries; none found a positive relationship. Of the 10 analyses of "other crime" (e.g., vandalism, disorderly conduct), 3 found a significant inverse relationship between the legal drinking age and crime; none found a positive relationship. Only 1 study that analyzed the relationship between social, academic and employment problems was of higher methodological quality, and it did not find any statistically significant results. Similarly, only 3 studies that analyzed "other problems" (e.g., alcoholism, cirrhosis, general alcohol-related problems) were of higher quality, and none found significant results.

Mediating Factors

In addition to studies specifically evaluating the effects of minimum drinking age policies, there is a growing scientific literature on several closely related factors that can be deemed to mediate the relationship between drinking age law and outcomes of interest. Such factors include self-reported ease of access to alcohol and sources of alcohol, purchase success rates by underage-appearing buyers at bars and liquor stores, use of false age-identification documents, patterns of enforcement of the drinking age, effects of enforcement "crackdowns" and use of home delivery as a source of alcohol for youth. We identified 34 published articles on these factors; findings are summarized in Table 4.

Results show that more than half to more than three-quarters of teens surveyed report that alcohol is easy to obtain. Moreover, explicit tests of the propensity of alcohol retailers to sell to underage youth in purchase-attempt studies found 44-97% of outlets tested sold to underage youth with no request for age identification. Such studies show that the beneficial effects of the age-21 policy to date in terms of reduced drinking and reduced traffic crashes among youth have largely been achieved with minimal implementation of the law.

Most studies reveal that use of false age identification documents is only a modest problem. Estimates range from 5% to 21% of teens report using false age identification to obtain alcohol. One study was an outlier, finding 46% of undergraduate sociology students on one campus report use of false age identification (Durkin et al., 1996). Most studies on use of false age identification to date have not specifically focused on college students. It is possible that the accessibility and use of false identification documents is higher in college environments, but we do not know whether this is the case based on currently available data.

The limited degree to which age-21 policies have been implemented is also shown in several enforcement studies. Such studies have consistently found very low levels of enforcement of the age-21 policy. Enforcement actions against those selling or providing alcohol to minors are particularly rare (Wagenaar and Wolfson, 1994).

Studies of the effects of increased enforcement show it to be a highly effective means to reduce alcohol sales to minors. Increased enforcement, specifically compliance checks on retail alcohol outlets, typically cuts rates of sales to minors by at least half (Grube, 1997; Lewis et al., 1996; Preusser et al., 1994).

Finally, a recent study reports that 10% of high school seniors and 7% of 18- to 20-year olds use the home delivery services of alcohol retailers to obtain alcoholic beverages (Fletcher et al., 2000).

In summary, research on mediating factors between the establishment of a legal age for purchase and consumption of alcohol and actual effects on teen drinking and alcohol-related problems indicates clear means of further increasing the effectiveness of this policy. Such means include, most notably, increased rates of enforcement to prevent alcohol sales to minors. Other means to improve implementation of the age-21 policy, such as efforts to reduce use of false age identification and tighter restrictions on home delivery of alcohol, may also help enhance effectiveness of this law.

Conclusion

Compared with a wide range of other programs and efforts to reduce drinking among teenagers, increasing the legal age for purchase and consumption of alcohol to 21 appears to have been the most successful effort to date (compare studies summarized in Table 1 with studies cited in reviews of other prevention efforts such as Moskowitz [1989] and Gorman and Speer [1996]). The magnitude of effects of the age-21 policy may appear small, particularly in studies using weak research designs and having low levels of statistical power. However, even modest effects applied to the entire population of youth result in very large societal benefits. For example, the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, using an average estimated reduction in traffic fatalities due to the legal drinking age of 13%, calculates that the age-21 policy prevented 846 deaths in 1997 and prevented a total of 17,359 deaths since 1975 (National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1998).

A large proportion of studies of the MLDA found a statistically significant, inverse relationship between the MLDA and alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems (48% of the higher quality studies). Only a small number of studies found a statistically significant, positive relationship between the MLDA and various outcomes (1% of the higher quality studies). A large number of studies found no statistically significant relationship. In addition to differences in quality of research design and analyses, several other factors may account for variability in results across studies, including size of sample and extent of change in policy. The power to detect a statistically significant effect is directly influenced by the size of the sample. In some states, the MLDA was raised only 1 year, from age 20 to age 21; in other states it was raised from age 18 to 21. Studies of policy changes that affect smaller segments of the population may be less likely to detect effects simply because of reduced statistical power when analyzing fewer data. Given potential design and analysis limitations in any single study, the large proportion of MLDA studies that found a significant inverse relationship with various outcomes gives strong support for the effectiveness of the MLDA.

It is difficult to estimate accurately the effects of the drinking age specifically on college students. Unfortunately, most studies focusing on college students have been based on weaker cross-sectional designs or limited nonprobability samples. Only 9% of the college-specific studies (6 of 64) used a higher quality research design. Of these higher quality studies, none found a statistically significant inverse relationship between the MLDA and consumption or alcohol-related problems. In addition, of these 6 analyses, 4 included a sample of students at only one university. Although it is possible that the age-21 policy has been less effective on college campuses than among the general youth population, existing research clearly does not suggest that the age-21 MLDA has increased problems among college students. However, more studies that use robust research designs would be needed to assess accurately the effect of the MLDA specifically on college campuses. In addition, studies of potential mediating factors on campuses are also needed. For example, how well are MLDA laws enforced on college campuses? How easily can underage students obtain alcohol on and around campus? If one assumes that the MLDA is less effective on college campuses, perhaps it is due to lax enforcement and particularly easy access to alcohol by underage youth in such settings.

Finally, despite progress in recent decades, most youth continue to have access to alcohol, most drink at least occasionally, and a substantial fraction regularly become intoxicated. The social costs from injuries, deaths and damage associated with underage drinking remain high. The benefits of the legal drinking age of 21 have occurred with little or no active enforcement in most areas. Simply by increasing enforcement levels and deterring adults from selling or providing alcohol to minors, even more injuries and deaths related to alcohol use among youth are likely to be prevented each year.

Policy Issues Related to the Minimum Legal Drinking Age

Despite an abundance of research demonstrating the effectiveness of the age-21 MLDA in reducing youth drinking and alcohol-related problems, three decades after states first began lowering minimum drinking ages, and two decades after states were in the midst of raising their legal drinking ages, a few states are again considering lowering their legal age limits for drinking. Many issues and arguments heard decades ago are again occasionally heard (Fell, 1985; Toomey et al., 1996). One difference this time around is that we have the benefit of hundreds of research studies summarized in the body of this article. Here we summarize 13 similar issues that are still often raised in policy debates by those opposed to a legal drinking age of 21 and provide up-to-date responses that may be useful to college administrators, public health practitioners and others.

Issue 1

Issue. "Establishing a legal drinking age of 21 is unconstitutional age discrimination."

Response. This question has been treated in detail in two court cases. The first case challenged, in federal court, the constitutionality of Michigan's increase in the drinking age, one of the early states to raise the legal age back to 21 (Guy, 1978). The court ruled, on the basis of scientific evidence that linked lower drinking ages to increased traffic crash involvement among youth, that the drinking-age discrimination was reasonably related to the state objective of reducing highway crashes. Thus the higher drinking age withstood the constitutional challenge on three key legal issues: (1) drinking alcohol is not a "fundamental" right guaranteed by the Constitution, (2) age is not inherently a "suspect" criteria for discrimination (in contrast to race or ethnicity, for example) and (3) using the drinking age to prevent highway crashes has a "rational basis" in available scientific evidence. The court mentioned additional reasons that a higher drinking age is not unconstitutional. The higher drinking age does not cause a permanent disability, but is only a temporary postponement of a specific behavior for the young person's own protection. Furthermore, states have broad powers to regulate the distribution and use of beverage alcohol under the Twenty-first Amendment, which ended Prohibition. Therefore, the drinking age, like other alcohol-control regulations, has a "strong presumption of validity," according to the court.

More recently, the state of Louisiana's age-21 MLDA was challenged in court on the premise that it violated the state's constitutional law regarding age discrimination. Louisiana's State Supreme Court concluded, however, that "statutes establishing the minimum drinking age at a level higher than the age of majority are not arbitrary because they substantially further the appropriate governmental purpose of improving highway safety, and thus are constitutional" (Manuel vs State of Louisiana, 1996). In other words, because the age-21 MLDA was based on empirical evidence that the law saved lives, the court decided that it was not an arbitrary law and thus did not violate Louisiana's constitution.

Issue 2

Issue. "The federal government is exerting too much power over the states. The federal law encouraging states to set the legal drinking age at 21, by withholding highway funds from states that do not do so, is just one example of this."

Response. Citizens groups in a number of states began the initial movement to raise the drinking age to 21. Numerous statewide and national surveys show overwhelming public support for the drinking age of 21, both in the late 1970s and early 1980s when states were raising the age (Wagenaar, 1993), and today. The most recent national survey shows 84% of the U.S. population age 18 and over oppose lowering the age from 21 to 19 (Wagenaar et al., 2000). As several states increased the drinking age to 21, significant reductions in multiple types of injuries (including deaths related to car crashes) were observed (Jones et al., 1992; Wagenaar, 1993). However, as some states raised their drinking age while neighboring states did not, some young people drove across state lines to get alcohol, increasing the chance of traffic crashes. Recognizing that having a uniform drinking age achieves safety, the federal government strongly encouraged, but did not mandate, the remaining states to increase their drinking ages to age 21.

Issue 3

Issue. "Europeans teens are allowed to drink from an early age, yet those countries don't have the alcohol-related problems we do. What we need are fewer restrictions, not more."

Response. The idea that Europeans do not have alcohol-related problems is a myth. European youth may be at less risk of traffic crashes because youth drive less frequently in Europe than in the United States. Compared with the United States, Europeans have higher legal driving ages, more expensive automobiles and greater access to public transportation. Looking beyond traffic crashes, however, European countries have similar or higher rates of other alcohol-related problems compared with the United States. For example, in 1990, France and Italy had higher per capita alcohol consumption and higher rates of cirrhosis deaths than did the United States. Per capita consumption in France and Italy was 12.7 and 8.7 liters of alcohol, respectively, compared with 7.5 in the United States. Cirrhosis death rates in France and Italy were 26.8 and 17.0 per 100,000, respectively, whereas the U.S. rate was 11.6 (Edwards et al., 1994). European countries are now looking to the United States for research and experience regarding the age-21 policy. Europeans are initiating the debate on the most appropriate age for legal access to alcohol.

Issue 4

Issue. "If I'm old enough to go to war, I should be old enough to drink."

Response. Many rights have different ages of initiation. A person can obtain a hunting license at age 12, driver's license at age 16, vote and serve in the military at 18, serve in the U.S. House of Representatives at age 25 and in the U.S. Senate at age 30 and run for President at age 35. Other rights we regulate include the sale and use of tobacco and legal consent for sexual intercourse and marriage. The minimum age of initiation is based on the specific behavior involved and must take into account the dangers and benefits of that behavior at a given age (Fell, 1985). The age-21 policy for alcohol takes into account the fact that underage drinking is related to numerous serious problems, including injuries and deaths resulting from car crashes, suicide, homicide, assault, drowning and recreational injuries. In fact, the leading cause of death among teens is car crashes (National Center for Health Statistics, 1994), and alcohol is involved in approximately one-third of these deaths (National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1998).

Issue 5

Issue. "Nineteen- and twenty-year-olds are drinking anyway. If we legalize it, at least they'll be drinking in a controlled supervised settings, such as a bar or nightclub, rather than in cars or at unsupervised parties."

Response. Data show bars and nightclubs are not safe, controlled locations. Studies have repeatedly shown a majority of alcohol outlets regularly break the law, for example, by selling alcohol to minors (Forster et al., 1994, 1995; Preusser and Williams, 1992) or selling to intoxicated patrons (Toomey et al., 1999). When the legal age is lower than 21, teens purchase the majority of their alcohol at liquor stores because it is cheaper than getting it at bars. They then consume this alcohol in homes, cars or parks. These areas are very difficult to control (Fell, 1985).

There is also some "trickle-down" effect in that when youth get alcohol they often give it to even younger teens (Jones-Webb et al., 1997a). When the legal age is 21, 19- and 20-year olds can often obtain alcohol from their friends. When the drinking age was 18 and 19, 17- and even 16-year olds were often able to get alcohol from their friends. If the drinking age is lower, more alcohol will be available to younger high school students and perhaps even middle school students. There will always be some people who violate laws, but this does not mean we should condone the illegal behavior by modifying the law. The age-21 policy has resulted in a reduction in the amount of alcohol consumed and a substantial decrease in the number of car crashes involving underage drinkers. These results have occurred despite the fact that the law is often not strictly enforced (Wagenaar and Wolfson, 1994, 1995).

Issue 6

Issue. "Lower rates of alcohol-related crashes among 19- to 20-year olds aren't related to the age-21 policy, but rather they're related to increased drinking-driving education efforts, tougher enforcement and tougher drunk-driving penalties."

Response. After the age-21 MLDA was implemented, alcohol-involved highway crashes declined immediately (i.e., starting the next month) among the 18- to 20-year-old population. Careful research has shown declines are not due to enforcement of and tougher penalties for driving while intoxicated, but are directly a result of the legal drinking age. Studies have also shown that education alone is not effective at reducing youth drinking (Clayton et al., 1996; Ellickson et al., 1993). To achieve long-term reductions in youth drinking problems, we have to change the environment by making alcohol less accessible to teens.

Issue 7

Issue. "Making it illegal to drink until 21 just increases the desire for the 'forbidden fruit.' Then, when teens turn 21, they'll drink even more."

Response. Actually, the opposite is true. Early legal access to alcohol is associated with higher rates of drinking as an adult. When the drinking age is 21, those under 21 drink less and continue to drink less through their early twenties. The lower rates of drinking and the reductions in injury and death before age 21 are not compensated for after reaching 21 with rates higher than they would have been (O'Malley and Wagenaar, 1991).

Issue 8

Issue. "Who will pay for enforcement of these laws? The age-21 law is too expensive."

Response. We already pay large portions of our tax dollars for problems resulting from alcohol. For example, in Minnesota, cities use approximately one-third of their police budgets to deal with alcohol-related problems (Cities Bulletin, 1989); in the United States, we pay more than $10 billion annually just for the costs associated with drunk driving (Kenkel, 1993a). Moreover, drinkers clearly do not pay their own way. They end up generating costs (in health care costs, legal fees and lost wages) of more than a dollar for every drink sold—costs we all pay in increased taxes, higher health and auto insurance premiums and higher costs for goods and services (Miller and Blincoe, 1994). The higher drinking age saves money by resulting in fewer alcohol-related health problems, fewer alcohol-related injuries and less vandalism.

Issue 9

Issue. "We have other more important problems to deal with. The truth is, underage drinking is just not a big problem."

Response. Underage drinking is a serious problem. In 1998, 52% of high school seniors in the United States drank alcohol in the last month, and more than 30% were intoxicated at least once in the last 2 weeks (Johnston et al., 1998). And these are the lower numbers under the age-21 policy. Teens would be drinking even more if the legal age were lowered. A recent national survey indicates that 96% of the public remains concerned about teen drinking (Wagenaar et al., 2000). The age-21 law clearly does not eliminate youth drinking, but it is one important component of a multifaceted effort to minimize youth drinking problems.

Issue 10

Issue. "Here come the Prohibitionists."

Response. Those supporting the age-21 policy are not Prohibitionists. They are not interested in outlawing all alcohol consumption for adults and are not interested in putting the alcohol industry out of business. They are interested in protecting youth and the safety of all citizens in our communities by supporting implementation and enforcement of the law that states that it is illegal to sell alcohol to those under the age of 21. They are interested in protecting property and reducing the costs spent on health care and crime. These are goals shared by most of the public, and research shows that if we can reduce youth access to alcohol, we can help achieve these goals.

Issue 11

Issue. "We need to punish those teens who are drinking and creating problems, not enact policies that will affect the whole community."

Response. This problem requires shared responsibility. It is adults who create the environments within communities that provide youth with easy access to alcohol. Adults own and operate the businesses that sell alcohol to underage youth. Adults permit advertising and marketing of alcohol in ways that appeal to teens. Thus it is not appropriate to blame just the teens for drinking. Surely, teens have a responsibility not to attempt to purchase or consume alcohol. But arresting after the fact and labeling as criminals teens who drink is not the most effective approach. A modest civil penalty for the teenager caught with alcohol is appropriate.

More effective in the long term are efforts to reduce the supply of alcohol to teens to prevent youth drinking and the resulting tragedies before they happen. This requires active enforcement of statutes and regulations on those who sell or provide alcohol to teens, with appropriate penalties for violations.

Issue 12

Issue. "We drank when we were young and we grew out of it. It's just a phase that all teens go through."

Response. Unfortunately, many teens will not "grow out of it." Studies indicate that youth who start drinking before they are 21 are more likely to drink heavier later in life, whereas those who do not drink until they are 21 tend to drink less as adults (Grant and Dawson, 1997). Teens who drink are also more likely to try other illegal drugs and to become victims of crime (Kandel et al., 1992). If we accept that teen drinking is just a normal phase that teens go through, youth will continue to experience car crashes, other injuries, early unprotected sex and other common problems associated with drinking.

Issue 13

Issue. "If teens can't get alcohol, they'll just switch to other, perhaps even more dangerous, drugs."

Response. Research shows that the opposite is true; teens who drink and/or smoke are more likely also to use other drugs (Fell, 1985; Kandel et al., 1992). If we can keep youth from using alcohol and tobacco, we can actually reduce the chance that they will try other illegal drugs. Moreover, when the drinking age was raised to 21, and teen drinking declined, there was no evidence of a compensatory increase in other drug use (O'Malley and Wagenaar, 1991).

References

Arndt, S.K. Adolescent Alcohol Consumption and a Changing Legal Drinking Age. Dissertation Abstracts Int. 50 (4-B): 1308, 1989.

Asch, P. and Levy, D.T. Does the minimum drinking age affect traffic fatalities? J. Policy Anal. Manage. 6: 180-192, 1987.

Asch, P. and Levy, D.T. Young driver fatalities: The roles of drinking age and drinking experience. So. Econ. J. 57: 512-520, 1990.

Bako, G., Mackenzie, W. and Smith, E.S. Effect of legislated lowering of the drinking age on fatal highway accidents among young drivers in Alberta, 1970-1972. Can. J. Publ. Hlth 67: 161-163, 1976.

Barsby, S.L. and Marshall, G.L. Short-term consumption effects of a lower minimum alcohol-purchasing age. J. Stud. Alcohol 38: 1665-1679, 1977.

Bellows, D.C. The Effects of Lowering the Drinking Age in Nebraska: A Quasi-Experimental Time-Series Analysis, Ph.D. dissertation, Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska-Lincoln, 1980.

Bessmer, M.A. Relationship of Self-Reported Problems and Drinking Among College Students. Dissertation Abstracts Int. 45 (9-B): 3061, 1985.

Birckmayer, J. and Hemenway, D. Minimum-age drinking laws and youth suicide, 1970-1990. Amer. J. Publ. Hlth 89: 1365-1368, 1999.

Breed, W., Wallack, L. and Grube, J.W. Alcohol advertising in college newspapers: A 7-year follow-up. J. Amer. Coll. Hlth 38: 255-262, 1990.

Brown, D.B. and Maghsoodloo, S. Study of alcohol involvement in young driver accidents with the lowering of the legal age of drinking in Alabama. Accid. Anal. Prev. 13: 319-322, 1981.

Casswell, S. and Zhang, J.F. Access to alcohol from licensed premises during adolescence: A longitudinal study. Addiction 92: 737-745, 1997.

Chaloupka, F.J., Saffer, H. and Grossman, M. Alcohol-control policies and motor-vehicle fatalities. J. Legal Stud. 22: 161-186, 1993.

Chung, H.B. Drunk Driving and Social Control Policies: Econometric Analysis of Policy Effectiveness. Dissertation Abstracts Int. 57 (12A): 5291, 1997.

Cities Bulletin (newsletter). St. Paul, MN: League of Minnesota Cities, Vol. 30, November 9, 1989.

Clayton, R.R., Cattarello, A.M. and Johnston, B.M. The effectiveness of drug abuse resistance education (Project DARE): 5-year follow-up results. Prev. Med. 25: 307-318, 1996.

Coate, D. and Grossman, M. Change in alcoholic beverage prices and legal drinking ages: Effects on youth alcohol use and motor vehicle mortality. Alcohol Hlth Res. World 12: 22-25, 1987.

Coate, D. and Grossman, M. Effects of alcoholic beverage prices and legal drinking ages on youth alcohol use. J. Law Econ. 31: 145-171, 1988.

Colon, I. Alcohol Control Policies and Their Relation to Alcohol Consumption and Alcoholism, Ph.D. dissertation, Waltham, MA: F. Heller Graduate School, Brandeis University, 1980.

Colon, I. Alcohol beverage purchase age and single-vehicle highway fatalities. J. Safety Res. 15: 159-162, 1984.

Colon, I. and Cutter, H.S. Relationship of beer consumption and state alcohol and motor vehicle policies to fatal accidents. J. Safety Res. 14: 83-89, 1983.

Cook, P.J. and Tauchen, G. The effect of minimum drinking age legislation on youthful auto fatalities, 1970-1977. J. Legal Stud. 13: 169-190, 1984.

Davis, J.E. and Reynolds, N.C. Alcohol use among college students: Responses to raising the purchase age. J. Amer. Coll. Hlth 38: 263-269, 1990.

Decker, M.D., Graitcer, P.L. and Schaffner, W. Reduction in motor vehicle fatalities associated with an increase in the minimum drinking age. JAMA 260: 3604-3610, 1988.

Dee, T.S. State alcohol policies, teen drinking and traffic fatalities. J. Publ. Econ. 72: 289-315, 1999.

Douglas, R.L. and Millar, C.W. Alcohol availability and alcohol-related casualties in Michigan 1968-1976. In: Galanter, M. (Ed.) Currents in Alcoholism, Vol. 6: Treatment and Rehabilitation and Epidemiology, New York: Grune & Stratton, 1979, pp. 303-317.

Du Mouchel, W., Williams, A.F. and Zador, P. Raising the alcohol purchase age: Its effects on fatal motor vehicle crashes in twenty-six states. J. Legal Stud. 16: 249-266, 1987.

Durant, R.F. and Legge, J.S. Policy design, social regulation, and theory building: Lessons from the traffic safety policy arena. Polit. Res. Q. 46: 641-656, 1993.

Durkin, K.F., Wolfe, T.W. and Phillips, D.W., 3rd. College students' use of fraudulent identification to obtain alcohol: An exploratory analysis. J. Alcohol Drug Educ. 41: 92-104, 1996.

Edwards, G., Anderson, P., Babor, T.F., Casswell, S., Ferrence, R., Giesbrecht, N., Godfrey, C., Holder, H.D., Hemmens, P., Mäkelä, K., Midanik, L.T., Norström, T., Österberg, E., Romelsjö, A., Room, R., Simpura, J. and Skog, O.-J. Alcohol Policy and the Public Good, New York: Oxford Univ. Press, 1994.

Ellickson, P.L., Bell, R.M. and Harrison, E.R. Changing adolescent propensities to use drugs: Results from project ALERT. Hlth Educ. Q. 20: 227-242, 1993.

Engs, R.C. and Hanson, D.J. Age-specific alcohol prohibition and college students' drinking problems. Psychol. Rep. 59 (2, Pt 2): 979-984, 1986.

Engs, R.C. and Hanson, D.J. University students' drinking patterns and problems: Examining the effects of raising the purchase age. Publ. Hlth Rep. 103: 667-673, 1988.

Fell, J.C. Drinking age 21: Facts, myths and fictions. Amer. Assoc. Auto. Med. Q. 7: 21-26, 1985.

Ferreira, J. and Sicherman, A. Impact of Massachusetts' reduced drinking age on auto accidents. Accid. Anal. Prev. 8: 229-239, 1976.

Figlio, D.N. The effect of drinking age laws and alcohol-related crashes: Time-series evidence from Wisconsin. J. Policy Anal. Manage. 14: 555-566, 1995.

Fletcher, L.A., Toomey, T.L., Wagenaar, A.C., Short, B. and Willenbring, M.L Alcohol home delivery services: A source of alcohol for underage drinkers. J. Stud. Alcohol 61: 81-84, 2000.

Forster, J.L., McGovern, P.G., Wagenaar, A.C., Wolfson, M., Perry, C.L. and Anstine, P.S. The ability of young people to purchase alcohol without age identification in northeastern Minnesota, USA. Addiction 89: 699-705, 1994.

Forster, J.L., Murray, D.M., Wolfson, M. and Wagenaar, A.C. Commercial availability of alcohol to young people: Results of alcohol purchase attempts. Prev. Med. 24: 342-347, 1995.

George, W.H., Crowe, L.C., Abwender, D. and Skinner, J.B. Effects of raising the drinking age to 21 years in New York State on self-reported consumption by college students. J. Appl. Social Psychol. 19 (8, Pt 1): 623-635, 1989.

Goldsmith, H. Illicit alcohol procurement by underage persons: Prevention implications and strategies for melioration. J. Offend. Counsel. Serv. Rehab. 13: 133-162, 1988.

Gonzalez, G.M. Effects of raising the drinking age among college students in Florida. Coll. Student J. 23: 67-75, 1989.

Gonzalez, G.M. Effects of raising the drinking age and related campus initiatives on student alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems. J. Coll. Student Devel. 31: 181-183, 1990a.

Gonzalez, G.M. Effects of drinking age on reduced consumption of alcohol reported by college students: 1981-1986. J. Drug Issues 20: 67-73, 1990b.

Gordon, R.A. and Minor, S.W. Attitudes toward a change in the legal drinking age: Reactance versus compliance. J. Coll. Student Devel. 33: 171-176, 1992.

Gorman, D.M. and Speer, P.W. Preventing alcohol abuse and alcohol-related problems through community intervention: A review of evaluation studies. Psychol. Hlth 11: 95-131, 1996.

Grant, B.F. and Dawson, D.A. Age at onset of alcohol use and its association with DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: Results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J. Subst. Abuse 9: 103-110, 1997.

Grossman, M., Chaloupka, F.J., Saffer, H. and Laixuthai, A. Effects of alcohol price policy on youth: A summary of economic research. In: Boyd, G.M., Howard, J. and Zucker, R.A. (Eds.) Alcohol Problems Among Adolescents: Current Directions in Prevention Research, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1995, pp. 225-242.

Grube, J.W. Preventing sales of alcohol to minors: Results from a community trial. Addiction 92 (Suppl. 2): S251-S260, 1997.

Guy, R. Opinion. Ref. Civil No. 8-73015 and Civil No. 8-73159. U.S. District Court, Eastern District of Michigan, Southern Division, Detroit, MI, December 22, 1978.

Hingson, R., Merrigan, D. and Heeren, T. Effects of Massachusetts raising its legal drinking age from 18 to 20 on deaths from teenage homicide, suicide, and nontraffic accidents. Pediat. Clin. No. Amer. 32: 221-232, 1985.

Hingson, R.W., Scotch, N., Mangione, T., Meyers, A., Glantz, L., Heeren, T., Lin, N., Mucatel, M. and Pierce, G. Impact of legislation raising the legal drinking age in Massachusetts from 18 to 20. Amer. J. Publ. Hlth 73: 163-170, 1983.

Hoadley, J.F., Fuchs, B.C. and Holder, H.D. Effect of alcohol beverage restrictions on consumption: A 25-year longitudinal analysis. Amer. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 10: 375-401, 1984.

Hoskin, A.F., Yalung-Mathews, D. and Carraro, B.A. Effect of raising the legal minimum drinking age on fatal crashes in 10 states. J. Safety Res. 17: 117-121, 1986.

Howland, J., Birckmayer, J., Hemenway, D. and Cote, J. Did changes in minimum age drinking laws affect adolescent drowning (1970-90)? Injury Prev. 4: 288-291, 1998.

Hughes, S.P. and Dodder, R.A. Raising the legal minimum drinking age: Short-term effects with college student samples. J. Drug Issues 16: 609-620, 1986.

Hughes, S.P. and Dodder, R.A. Changing the legal minimum drinking age: Results of a longitudinal study. J. Stud. Alcohol 53: 568-575, 1992.

Johnson, J.A., Oksanen, E.H., Veall, M.R. and Fretz, D. Short-run and long-run elasticities for Canadian consumption of alcoholic beverages: An error-correction mechanism/cointegration approach. Rev. Econ. Stat. 74: 64-74, 1992.

Johnston, L.D., O'Malley, P.M. and Bachman, J.G. National Survey Results on Drug Use from the Monitoring the Future Study, 1975-1997. Vol. 1 (NIH Publication No. 98-4345) and Vol. 2 (NIH Publication No. 98-4346), Washington: Government Printing Office, 1998.

Joksch, H.C. and Jones, R.K. Changes in the drinking age and crime. J. Crim. Justice 21: 209-221, 1993.

Jones, N.E., Pieper, C.F. and Robertson, L.S. Effect of legal drinking age on fatal injuries of adolescents and young adults. Amer. J. Publ. Hlth 82: 112-115, 1992.

Jones-Webb, R., Toomey, T., Miner, K., Wagenaar, A.C., Wolfson, M. And Poon, R. Why and in what context adolescents obtain alcohol from adults: A pilot study. Subst. Use Misuse 32: 219-228, 1997a.

Jones-Webb, R., Toomey, T.L., Short, B., Murray, D.M., Wagenaar, A. And Wolfson, M. Relationships among alcohol availability, drinking location, alcohol consumption, and drinking problems in adolescents. Subst. Use Misuse 32: 1261-1285, 1997b.

Kandel, D.B., Yamaguchi, K. and Chen, K. Stages of progression in drug involvement from adolescence to adulthood: Further evidence for the gateway theory. J. Stud. Alcohol 53: 447-457, 1992.

Kenkel, D.S. Do drunk drivers pay their way? A note on optimal penalties for drunk driving. J. Hlth Econ. 12: 136-149, 1993a.

Kenkel, D.S. Drinking, driving, and deterrence: The effectiveness and social costs of alternative policies. J. Law Econ. 36: 877-913, 1993b.

King, R.F. and Dudar, D. M. The politics of denial: The use of funding penalties as an implementation device for social policy. Policy Sci. 20: 307-337, 1987.

Klepp, K.I., Aas, H.N. and Waahlberg, R.B. Perceived access to alcohol among lower secondary school students. A longitudinal study (Norwegian). Tidsskr. Nor. Laegeforen. 116: 3132-3135, 1996a.

Klepp, K.I., Schmid, L.A. and Murray, D.M. Effects of the increased minimum drinking age law on drinking and driving behavior among adolescents. Addict. Res. 4: 237-244, 1996b.

Laixuthai, A. Youth, Alcohol Use and Abuse: An Examination of Public Policy and Laws on Alcohol, Marijuana, and Drunk Driving, Ph.D. dissertation, Chicago, IL: Health Sciences Center, University of Illinois at Chicago, 1994.

Laixuthai, A. and Chaloupka, F.J. Youth alcohol use and public policy. Contemp. Policy Issues 11: 70-81, 1993.

Legge, J.S., Jr. Reforming highway safety in New York State: An evaluation of alternative policy interventions. Social Sci. Q. 71: 373-382, 1990.

Lewis, R.K., Paine-Andrews, A., Fawcett, S.B., Francisco, V.T., Richter, K.P., Copple, B. and Copple, J.E. Evaluating the effects of a community coalition's efforts to reduce illegal sales of alcohol and tobacco products to minors. J. Commun. Hlth 21: 429-436, 1996.

Lillis, R.P., Williams, T.P. and Williford, W.R. Impact of the 19-yearold drinking age in New York. In: Holder, H.D. (Ed.) Advances in Substance Abuse: Behavioral and Biological Research, Supplement 1: Control Issues in Alcohol Abuse Prevention: Strategies for States and Communities, Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 1987, pp. 133-146.

Loeb, P.D. The determination of automobile fatalities. J. Transport. Econ. Policy 21: 279-287, 1987.

Lonnstrom, D.A. Impact of Changes in the New York State Minimum Drinking Age Law upon the College Campus. Dissertation Abstracts Int. 45 (8-A): 2670, 1985.

Lotterhos, J.F., Glover, E.D., Holbert, D. and Barnes, R.C. Intentionality of college students regarding North Carolina's 21-year drinking age law. Int. J. Addict. 23: 629-647, 1988.

McCall, M. Accuracy in detecting potential underage drinkers: The role of contextual effects in proofing. J. Appl. Social Psychol. 23: 875-890, 1993.

McCall, M. The effects of physical attractiveness on gaining access to alcohol: When social policy meets social decision making. Addiction 92: 597-600, 1997.

McCall, M. Physical attractiveness, mood, and the decision to card for the purchase of alcohol: Evidence for a mood-management hypothesis. J. Appl. Social Psychol. 29: 1172-1190, 1999.

McCornac, D.C. and Filante, R.W. Demand for distilled spirits: An empirical investigation. J. Stud. Alcohol 45: 176-178, 1984.

McFadden, M. and Wechsler, H. Minimum drinking age laws and teenage drinking. Psychiat. Opin. 16 (3): 26-28, 1979.

MacKinnon, D.P. and Woodward, J.A. The impact of raising the minimum drinking age on driver fatalities. Int. J. Addict. 21: 1331-1338, 1986.

Maisto, S.A. and Rachal, J.V. Indications of the relationship among adolescent drinking practices, related behavior, and drinking-age laws. In: Wechsler, H. (Ed.) Minimum-Drinking-Age Laws: An Evaluation, Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, 1980, pp. 155-176.

Males, M.A. Minimum purchase age for alcohol and young-driver fatal crashes: A long-term view. J. Legal Stud. 15: 181-211, 1986.

Manuel vs State Of Louisiana, 692 So. 2d 320 (La. 1996).

Mayer, R.R., Forster, J.L., Murray, D.M. and Wagenaar, A.C. Social settings and situations of underage drinking. J. Stud. Alcohol 59: 207-215, 1998.

Miller, T.R. and Blincoe, L.J. Incidence and cost of alcohol-involved crashes in the United States. Accid. Anal. Prev. 26: 583-591, 1994.

Mooney, L.A. and Gramling, R. The differential effects of the minimum drinking age law. Sociol. Inquiry 63: 330-338, 1993.

Mooney, L.A., Gramling, R. and Forsyth, C. Legal drinking age and alcohol consumption. Deviant Behav. 13: 59-71, 1992.

Moskowitz, J.M. The primary prevention of alcohol problems: A critical review of the research literature. J. Stud. Alcohol 50: 54-88, 1989.

Naor, E.M. and Nashold, R.D. Teenage driver fatalities following reduction in the legal drinking age. J. Safety Res. 7: 74-79, 1975.

National Center For Health Statistics. Health, United States, 1993, DHHS Publication No. (PHS) 94-1232, Washington: Government Printing Office, 1994.

National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Traffic Safety Facts 1997: Alcohol, Report No. DOT HS 808 806, Washington: Department of Transportation, 1998.

O'Leary, D., Gorman, D.M. and Speer, P.W. The sale of alcoholic beverages to minors. Publ. Hlth Rep. 109: 816-818, 1994.

O'Malley, P.M. and Wagenaar, A.C. Effects of minimum drinking age laws on alcohol use, related behaviors, and traffic crash involvement among American youth: 1976-1987. J. Stud. Alcohol 52: 478-491, 1991.

Ornstein, S.I. A survey of findings on the economic and regulatory determinants of the demand for alcoholic beverages. Subst. Alcohol Actions/Misuse 5: 39-44, 1984.

Park, J. Solving the Problem of Alcohol-Impaired Driving: An Evaluation of Alcohol-Related Legal Policies in the States. Dissertation Abstracts Int. 54 (9-A): 3589, 1994.

Parker, R.N. Alcohol and Homicide: A Deadly Combination of Two American Traditions, Albany, NY: State Univ. of New York Press, 1995.

Perkins, H.W. and Berkowitz, A.D. Stability and contradiction in college students' drinking following a drinking-age law change. J. Alcohol Drug Educ. 35 (1): 60-77, 1989.

Preusser, D.F. and Williams, A.F. Sales of alcohol to underage purchasers in three New York counties and Washington, D.C. J. Publ. Hlth Policy 13: 306-317, 1992.

Preusser, D.F., Williams, A.F. and Weinstein, H.B. Policing underage alcohol sales. J. Safety Res. 25: 127-133, 1994.

Rooney, J.F. and Schwartz, S.M. The effect of minimum drinking age laws upon adolescent alcohol use and problems. Contemp. Drug Probl. 6: 569-583, 1977.

Rubington, E. Drinking in the dorms: A study of the etiquette of RAresident relations. J. Drug Issues 20: 451-461, 1990.

Ruhm, C.J. Alcohol policies and highway vehicle fatalities. J. Hlth Econ. 15: 435-454, 1996.

Saffer, H. and Grossman, M. Beer taxes, the legal drinking age, and youth motor vehicle fatalities. J. Legal Stud. 16: 351-374, 1987a.

Saffer, H. and Grossman, M. Drinking age laws and highway mortality rates: Cause and effect. Econ. Inquiry 25: 403-417, 1987b.

Schofield, M.J., Weeks, C. and Sanson-Fisher, R. Alcohol sales to minors: A surrogate study. Prev. Med. 23: 827-831, 1994.

Schwartz, R.H., Farrow, J.A., Banks, B. and Giesel, A.E. Use of false ID cards and other deceptive methods to purchase alcoholic beverages during high school. J. Addict. Dis. 17 (3): 25-33, 1998.

Schweitzer, S.O., Intriligator, M.D. and Salehi, H. Alcoholism: An econometric model of its causes, its effects and its control. In: Grant, M., Plant, M. and Williams, A. (Eds.) Economics and Alcohol: Consumption and Controls, New York: Gardner Press, 1983, pp. 107-127.

Smart, R.G. Changes in alcoholic beverage sales after reductions in the legal drinking age. Amer. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 4: 101-108, 1977.

Smart, R.G. and Adlaf, E.M. Age of majority cards and drinking among young people. J. Alcohol Drug Educ. 32 (3): 60-64, 1987.

Smart, R.G., Adlaf, E.M. and Walsh, G.W. Procurement of alcohol and underage drinking among adolescents in Ontario. J. Stud. Alcohol 57: 419-424, 1996.

Smart, R.G. and Finley, J. Changes in drinking age and per capita beer consumption in ten Canadian provinces. Addict. Dis. 2: 393-402, 1976.

Smart, R.G. and Schmidt, W. Drinking and problems from drinking after a reduction in the minimum drinking age. Brit. J. Addict. 70: 347-358, 1975.

Smith, D.I. Effect on non-traffic accident hospital admissions of lowering the drinking age in two Australian states. Contemp. Drug Probl. 13: 621-639, 1986.

Smith, D.I. and Burvill, P.W. Effect on traffic safety of lowering the drinking age in three Australian states. J. Drug Issues 16: 183-198, 1986.

Smith, R.A., Hingson, R.W., Morelock, S., Heeren,T., Mucatel, M., Mangione, T. and Scotch, N. Legislation raising the legal drinking age in Massachusetts from 18 to 20: Effect on 16 and 17 year-olds. J. Stud. Alcohol 45: 534-539, 1984.

Thiel, R.R. The Impact of the Raised Drinking Age in Texas on Alcohol-Related Traffic Accidents and Fatalities. Dissertation Abstracts Int. 46 (5-A): 1419, 1985.

Toomey, T.L., Rosenfeld, C. and Wagenaar, A.C. The minimum legal drinking age: History, effectiveness and ongoing debate. Alcohol Hlth Res. World 20: 213-218, 1996.

Toomey, T.L., Wagenaar, A.C., Kilian, G., Fitch, O., Rothstein, C. and Fletcher, L. Alcohol sales to pseudo-intoxicated bar patrons. Publ. Hlth Rep. 114: 337-342, 1999.

Vaucher, S., Rehm, J., Benvenuti, J. and Muller, R. Young teenagers and access to alcohol in a Swiss canton: Evidence from observational testing and from a telephone survey. Addiction 90: 1619-1625, 1995.

Vingilis, E. and Smart, R.G. Effects of raising the legal drinking age in Ontario. Brit. J. Addict. 76: 415-424, 1981.

Wagenaar, A.C. The Minimum Legal Drinking Age: A Time-Series Impact Evaluation. Dissertation Abstracts Int. 41 (8B): 2984, 1981.

Wagenaar, A.C. Aggregate beer and wine consumption: Effects of changes in the minimum legal drinking age and a mandatory beverage container deposit law in Michigan. J. Stud. Alcohol 43: 469-487, 1982a.

Wagenaar, A.C. Public policy effects on alcohol consumption in Maine and New Hampshire: 1970-1980. Contemp. Drug Probl. 11: 3-20, 1982b.

Wagenaar, A.C. Alcohol, Young Drivers, and Traffic Accidents: Effects of Minimum-Age Laws, Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, 1983a.

Wagenaar, A.C. Raising the legal drinking age in Maine: Impact on traffic accidents among young drivers. Int. J. Addict. 18: 365-377, 1983b.

Wagenaar, A.C. Preventing highway crashes by raising the legal minimum age for drinking: The Michigan experience 6 years later. J. Safety Res. 17: 101-109, 1986.

Wagenaar, A.C. Minimum drinking age and alcohol availability to youth: Issues and research needs. In: Hilton, M.E. and Bloss, G. (Eds.) Economics and the Prevention of Alcohol-Related Problems. NIAAA Research Monograph No. 25, NIH Publication No. 93-3513, Rockville, MD: Department of Health and Human Services, 1993, pp. 175-200.

Wagenaar, A.C., Finnegan, J.R., Wolfson, M., Anstine, P.S., Williams, C.L. and Perry, C.L. Where and how adolescents obtain alcoholic beverages. Publ. Hlth Rep. 108: 459-464, 1993.

Wagenaar, A.C., Harwood, E.H., Toomey, T.L., Denk, C.E. and Zander, K.M. Public opinion on alcohol policies in the United States: Results from a national survey. J. Publ. Hlth Policy 21: 303-327, 2000.

Wagenaar, A.C. and Maybee, R.G. The legal minimum drinking age in Texas: Effects of an increase from 18 to 19. J. Safety Res. 17: 165-178, 1986.

Wagenaar, A.C. and Toomey, T.L. Alcohol policy: Gaps between legislative action and current research. Contemp. Drug Probl. 27: 681-733, 2000.

Wagenaar, A.C., Toomey, T.L., Murray, D.M., Short, B.J., Wolfson, M. and Jones-Webb, R. Sources of alcohol for underage drinkers. J. Stud. Alcohol 57: 325-333, 1996.

Wagenaar, A.C. and Wolfson, M. Enforcement of the legal minimum drinking age in the United States. J. Publ. Hlth Policy 15: 37-53, 1994.

Wagenaar, A.C. and Wolfson, M. Deterring sales and provision of alcohol to minors: A study of enforcement in 295 counties in four states. Publ. Hlth Rep. 110: 419-427, 1995.

Weinstein, B.G. Costs and benefits of establishing a national purchasing age of 21. In: Holder, H.D. (Ed.) Advances in Substance Abuse: Behavioral and Biological Research, Supplement 1: Control Issues in Alcohol Abuse Prevention: Strategies for States and Communities, Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 1987, pp. 105-118.

Whitehead, P.C., Craig, J., Langford, N., MacArthur, C., Stanton, B. and Ferrence, R.G. Collision behavior of young drivers: Impact of the change in the age of majority. J. Stud. Alcohol 36: 1208-1223, 1975.

Wilkinson, J.T. Reducing drunken driving: Which policies are most effective? So. Econ. J. 54: 322-334, 1987.

Williams, A.F., Rich, R.F., Zador, P.L. and Robertson, L.S. The legal minimum drinking age and fatal motor vehicle crashes. J. Legal Stud. 4: 219-239, 1975.

Williams, A.F., Zador, P.L., Harris, S.S. and Karpf, R.S. The effect of raising the legal minimum drinking age on involvement in fatal crashes. J. Legal Stud. 12: 169-179, 1983.

Williams, T.P. and Lillis, R.P. Changes in alcohol consumption by 18-year-olds following an increase in New York State's purchase age to 19. J. Stud. Alcohol 47: 290-296, 1986.

Williams, T.P. and Lillis, R.P. Long-term changes in reported alcohol purchasing and consumption following an increase in New York State's purchase age to 19. Brit. J. Addict. 83: 209-217, 1988.

Williford, W.R. Effect of the Nineteen Alcohol Purchase Age on an Eighteen-Year-Old High-Risk Behavior Group. Dissertation Abstracts Int. 48 (A): 2746, 1988.

Wolfson, M., Toomey, T.L., Forster, J.L., Wagenaar, A.C., McGovern, P.G. and Perry, C.L. Characteristics, policies and practices of alcohol outlets and sales to underage persons. J. Stud. Alcohol 57: 670-674, 1996a.

Wolfson, M., Toomey, T.L., Murray, D.M., Forster, J.L., Short, B.J. And Wagenaar, A.C. Alcohol outlet policies and practices concerning sales to underage people. Addiction 91: 589-602, 1996b.

Wolfson, M., Wagenaar, A.C. and Hornseth, G.W. Law officers' views on enforcement of the minimum drinking age: A four-state study. Publ. Hlth Rep. 110: 428-438, 1995.

Yu, J. Alcohol purchase age laws and the serial beginning drinker in New York. Int. J. Addict. 30: 1289-1301, 1995.

Yu, J. Perceived parental/peer attitudes and alcohol-related behaviors: An analysis of the impact of the drinking age law. Subst. Use Misuse 33: 2687-2702, 1998.

Yu, J. and Shacket, R.W. Long-term change in underage drinking and impaired driving after the establishment of drinking age laws in New York State. Alcsm Clin. Exp. Res. 22: 1443-1449, 1998.


†Alexander C. Wagenaar may be reached at the above address or via email at: wagenaar@epi.umn.edu.

Last reviewed: 9/23/2005


Home
About Us
Awards
Site Map
FAQ
Accessibility
Plug-Ins
Privacy Policy
Contact Us
Web site Policies
Disclaimer

National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism
National Institute of Health
Freedom of Information Act
U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services
USA.Gov