text-only page produced automatically by LIFT Text

Transcoder Skip all navigation and go to page contentSkip top navigation and go to directorate navigationSkip top navigation and go to page navigation
National Science Foundation
News
design element
News
For the News Media
Special Reports
Research Overviews
NSF-Wide Investments
Speeches & Lectures
Multimedia Gallery
Search Multimedia
Image
Video
Audio
More
Multimedia in the News
NSF Executive Staff
News Archive
 


Jumping Spider Mimics (Image 9)

Female (left) and male jumping spiders of an undentified species

A female (left) and male (right) jumping spider of an undentified species (probably in the genus Paraphiddipus) from a tropical dry forest in Costa Rica within the Palo Verde Biological Station. The green male with the much-larger chelicerae (jaws) was found guarding the female. [Image 9 of 12 related images. See Image 10.]

More about this Image
As part of her dissertation research, supported by a National Science Foundation graduate research fellowship, Lisa Taylor of Arizona State University's (ASU) School of Life Sciences studied mimicry in the jumping spider family Salticidae. Salticidae are the largest family of jumping spiders in the world.

In biology, mimicry is the similarity of one species to another. The mimicry may offer protection from predators for one or both of the species involved, and may help the mimic acquire food more easily. The mimicking species may be similar to the mimicked in appearance, behavior, sound, scent and location.

There are four types of mimicry used by spiders:

  • Batesian--When the mimic takes on characteristics of an organism that is harmful (may sting or bite) and bad tasting, like an ant or wasp, thus helping the spider avoid becoming a meal (Taylor believes that most of the jumping spiders she studied were Batesian mimics).


  • Wasmannian mimicry--When the mimic resembles an organism in order to come and go in the organism's territory undisturbed, and may or may not be harmful to the organism. For example, the ability to mimic ants (the technical term for this is myrmecomorphy), particularly those with nasty stings and bites, gives jumping spiders protection from the predators that feed on them such as birds, wasps and lizards. Mimicking ants also gives spiders access to areas where they can hunt and hide that typically would be off limits. Some spiders have the ability to smell like ants--ants use chemical signals to communicate. By mimicking the scent of ants, the spiders can roam freely in and out of ant nests, where they may live or feed on ant larva.


  • Peckhamian (or aggressive) mimicry--When an aggressive mimic imitates its prey so it may feed on it more easily. For example, the bolas spider hangs a glob of sticky glue in the air that contains a chemical that mimics the pheromone of a female moth. When male moths fly near to check out the potential "mate," they get stuck to the blob and become a meal for the spider.


  • Mullerian mimicry--When the mimic has distinctive, brightly striped patterns that resemble those of a toxic bee or wasp (although no toxic jumping spiders have been identified so far). For example, spiders that mimic the large, fuzzy and brightly colored velvet ant (which is actually a wingless wasp) are protected from potential predators because they associate bright-colored creatures with a painful sting and stay away.
Currently, Taylor is studying the courtship rituals of jumping spiders. In particular, she is interested in how animals use color to communicate. In her dissertation work at ASU, she is working with a group of colorfully ornamented jumping spiders (like the ones pictured here) in the genus Habronattus. She's trying to understand how they use color to advertise themselves to potential mates and competitors, as well as to deceive and avoid potential predators. (Date of Image: 2005-2010)

Credit: Lisa Taylor, Arizona State University

General Restrictions:
Images and other media in the National Science Foundation Multimedia Gallery are available for use in print and electronic material by NSF employees, members of the media, university staff, teachers and the general public. All media in the gallery are intended for personal, educational and nonprofit/non-commercial use only.

Images credited to the National Science Foundation, a federal agency, are in the public domain. The images were created by employees of the United States Government as part of their official duties or prepared by contractors as "works for hire" for NSF. You may freely use NSF-credited images and, at your discretion, credit NSF with a "Courtesy: National Science Foundation" notation. Additional information about general usage can be found in Conditions.

Also Available:
Download the high-resolution JPG version of the image. (1.7 MB)

Use your mouse to right-click (Mac users may need to Ctrl-click) the link above and choose the option that will save the file or target to your computer.

 

Print this page
Back to Top of page